Understanding and Teaching
Religious Belief Systems
in the 21st Century Missions
by Norman E. Allison
(Former President – Evangelical Missiological Society)
Published on Global Missiology, October, 2006, Featured
Article
Introduction and Background
My purpose in writing this paper
is to demonstrate the use of a systems model as a way
to enable both graduate and undergraduate students who are training for
cross-cultural ministry to use specific models
to examine the structural properties of religious belief systems. In
so doing I am trusting that some who read or hear this presentation will see this
subject as a valuable area of preparation that should be taught more widely in undergraduate
and graduate studies for prospective or current cross-cultural workers.
Through
the application of logical models to case studies, students are trained to understand how religious beliefs are structured
in the minds of people and how they express themselves in the daily lives and rituals of people in every part of
the world. The ultimate purpose of the course
is to train cross-cultural workers to be able to understand and to empathize more deeply with the people among whom
they will serve.
As
we teach this course at
Many theologians are concerned
that in any use of anthropological theory there is always
the danger of adopting a purely relativistic approach to the study of the
beliefs of a people. They are correct in being concerned...as
they should be for their own discipline as well;
however, from an evangelical Christian perspective, the overriding concern for
the undergirding application of biblical absolutes to
theoretical models should prevent this. In recent
years there has been concern expressed that missiologists have gone too far in
the application of anthropology to missiological issues. I do
not join in that concern. In fact, I
2
am more
concerned that many Christian workers, even cross-cultural workers, have little
or no interest or training in anthropology.
Whatever our training may
include, none of us can take our
assumptions for granted without
ongoing evaluation. Christian anthropologists,
must always apply biblical absolutes
in such a way that they will form the essential guidelines for any application
of theory. In the Religious Belief Systems course, students know, from the first few days of class, the explicit biblical
assumptions which will guide our study, and they are cautioned to question any movement away from those guidelines.
From another
direction, contemporary discussions in anthropological literature have expressed continuing concern with the imposition
of Western structures of understanding on
non-Western cultural data. In this course I work to see that emic1
structures are given a high priority,
and I encourage the primarily monocultural students to examine non-Western beliefs and values without the inherent
ethnocentrism so often seen in the history of their predecessors. In our discussions, I often ask
questions to see if their perception is being modified to consider non-Western alternatives in solutions to problems
in case studies. It is gratifying to
see, as the semester progresses, that there is movement away from monocultural assumptions and reactions.
The
history of anthropology is replete with numerous theories designed to help us understand the mosaic of peoples and their
cultures around the world. They tend to come and go with time. As for my own approach in dealing with the
complexities involved in communicating
the Gospel in other cultures, exposure to various alternatives has led me to adopt a mixture of theoretical approaches.
Although my perspective and my methodology may be seen as eclectic, I follow what is known as the general
systems theory2 approach to cultural analysis, modified by a functionalist3
mentality, a bit of symbolic and cognitive theory, and a few other strange ideas...all further molded by my
evangelical Christian worldview! So
if my approach may be a little difficult to classify at times, this is the reason!
It
may be worth mentioning here that in recent years, many anthropologists seem to
be moving away from understanding
the intricacies of culture through the study of structural systems. In fact, in our postmodern world, all theories,
definitions, and methodologies seem to be open to new interpretations. In an
article just published in the
3 monthly
news publication of the American Anthropological Association, under the
subtitle, Postmodern Suspicion of Structure, one
professor writes:
“...in anthropology, we see a
move away from the panoramic sweep of cultural systems,
whether of a positivist stripe or hermeneutic grandeur.
...the trend in anthropology is
away from cross-cultural comparison and a search for statistical
or other universals. How ironic that, as the forces (and structures) of globalization
increase, anthropology turns more toward local knowledge and practice
” (Maynard 2003:8 &11).
So,
as some behavioral scientists in this postmodern era move to change how they
and we should understand the societies of the world, there is still a strong
affinity among some of us for the use
of the “systems” approach in the study of religion and related beliefs and practices. The data in this paper follow that
approach.
In addition to the influences I have noted, my thinking
has also been challenged and shaped by the writings of
Christian anthropologists Alan Tippett, Paul Hiebert and Charles Kraft,
along with missiologist, David Hesselgrave and others. In fact, some of their
ideas are so mixed with mine,
I have difficulty in sorting
them all out as I teach this course and write this paper! But I will
attempt to give credit where I am aware of using source materials...at least
they will all be mentioned in the attached bibliography!
In
the specific application and integration of all these factors, I need to
further state that my own study of religious
belief systems began during the ten years I spent working among the Arabs of Jordan and
Later,
when I knew I was going to stay in the
4
differences.
More recently, he has expanded his work in his own book, Patterns of Thought
in
The Course
My
design for this course: Religious Belief Systems ("RBS" to students),
gives a background in theory and
then uses a simple beginning model developed for the study of religious beliefs in any culture. I have kept
this model simple because I want it to be useful to undergraduate students,
most of whom have limited experience in dealing with other cultures. It is also
easy to remember and apply in whatever cultural system they work with.
I should mention,
also, that what I am writing does not give an overview of the whole
course, but does give what I believe to be the core of what I am
teaching in that area. Later I will
list topics covered during the entire semester and this will indicate the
broader scope of the subject.
I emphasize in class
that when we deal with the subject of Religious Belief Systems, we are really dealing with the way people think.
We are not just examining beliefs and values,
even though that is an important part of our study. Our focus is on how these
are structured in the human
mind.4 I have worked
to simplify many of the intricacies related to the subject in order to focus on a way of communicating with
undergraduate students which will interest and enlighten them, and to give them
tools which may be developed further
as they work with the complex beliefs of societies around the world. Some of
them may question whether I have
been successful in this goal, but most of them do seem to express a high level of interest.
Early in the course
they read Paul Hiebert’s classic article, “The Flaw of the Excluded Middle.” (1982:35-47) and we discuss this in class.
Students begin to see, many for the
first time, that our Western two-tiered view of the
universe typically leaves out an entire
dimension in the worldview of people in non-Western cultures. The model given
by Hiebert in the original article
is titled “A Western Two-Tiered View ofReality.”
For
those of you who may not be familiar with this concept, the Evangelical
Dictionary of World Missions
describes it this way:
Hiebert
built his analysis on a two-dimensional matrix. The first
dimension is that of three
worlds or domains: 1) a seen world (that which is of this world and seen), 2) the unseen of this world
(that which is of this world but not seen), and 3) an unseen transempirical world (that which pertains to
heavens, hells, and other worlds).
The second dimension is that of two types of analogies people use to explain the powers around them: 1) an organic
analogy (powers are personal, e.g., gods and spirits) and 2) a mechanical
analogy (powers are impersonal, e.g., gravity and electricity).
Combining the seen/unseen/transempirical worlds and organic/mechanical analogies into a matrix, Hiebert's model highlighted
the difference between Westerners,
who tend to see only two worlds (the seen world and the transempirical world) and many non-Westerners
who recognize the middle world, comprised
of unseen powers (magical forces, evil eye, mana) and spirits which are very much a part of everyday human life (e.g., a
person is ill because of a curse or a spirit
attack). The blind spot in the Western worldview Hiebert labeled the flaw of the excluded middle (2000:363).
With conceptual models like this,
the students begin to realize that it is important to understand
that people in different societies actually do see the world around them very differently.
They also begin to comment that the work God has called them to do is a bit
more demanding than they had thought previously.
From this point, I introduce a very simple model based on
a three-level triangle which looks at the process of human thought as it moves
from sensory perception at the base through levels of
abstraction. The model is simple because I want to reiterate to
6
students that no matter how
complex the religious belief system they study may be, there will nearly always be two consistent themes. The
model looks like this:
FIG. 2
This model shows the students
that
1.
human
thought processes tend to move from the level of
“sensory perception” at the base to a more “abstract” category and then on to
higher levels of abstraction (more than two, but this keeps the
model simple).
2.
more
and more is explained by less and less as
one moves toward the more abstract upper point of the triangle.
As
I re-introduce these very fundamental concepts periodically, the students are
able to use this model as a basic
building block throughout the course.
Quite often in cultural
anthropology, we stress differences between people in different cultures
because monocultural people are prone to assume that other people are just
like they are, with a few variations.
This is one of the major reasons for using ”The
Flaw of the Excluded Middle”(Hiebert 1982:35-47) early in the course. This
often profoundly modifies student thinking about worldviews. In
addition, as different belief systems are studied, students
are presented with a number of case studies illustrating an unlimited
number of different ways people devise to organize their religious
beliefs. However, at a certain point in the course, similarities between Traditional
(or Folk, or
7
Primal)
beliefs and those of people who have been taught in a Western scientific
context need to be stressed as well. So I move on to a
variation of this model to make comparisons:
FIG.3
Then, as I help students think
through the implications of this model, I refer to the following
premises5:
1. The theoretical character of
thought in traditional belief systems is essentially the
same as that in Western scientific thinking. In
other words, “Common sense” in both systems
of thought (traditional and Western) is the simplest tool for dealing with circumstances
in everyday life. However, when problems are not solved with common
sense solutions, both systems allow for thinking to move into a “higher,” more
abstract level.
Model A, the Traditional Belief
System: Let me illustrate this with an example from Robin
Horton’s work among the Kalabari people of
8
category is
familiar to the Kalabari...they do have a “middle” level...so they think and interact
with the spirit world on a daily basis. The diviner often looks for a possible
causal relationship between the social lives of the individuals
[anger, envy, etc.]. His worldview is very holistic and it is his
responsibility to determine the cause of an illness or misfortune and
usually to prescribe the cure, so his solution is a move into the middle level.
So
what has been described here is a “jump,” in the minds of the diviner and the people involved, from common sense to “mystical
thinking” in the traditional or folk belief system. For this type of belief system, most answers are found in the “common
sense everyday thought modes,” but if solutions to problems are not found there,
it is not uncommon to make this jump.
Since the “mystical category” is so holistically entwined with the “common sense” level in
traditional systems, the transition to this level is normally not a difficult one to make.
Model
B, the Western scientific system:
Using Horton’s illustration to show similarity
between the two modes of thought, a physicist is pictured looking intently through his microscope. He is observing small,
fast-moving particles going through a sheet of metal foil in his laboratory experiment. These particles are too
small to analyze, so he needs to compare them with some known quantity. As he
observes, he sees that these puzzling
observations have a similarity to the movements of planets in a solar system.
In his thinking, he has shifted from
the observable but unknown, to an analogy made with a known quantity. He has, in fact, moved into the
first level of abstract theory to find an answer to his problem. There are differences between the two systems,
but the similarities in arriving at
solutions are basically the same.
2. The level of abstract theory
(high/low) used varies with the context.
An interesting aspect of this premise is that a
person’s choices, as he moves into the abstract
levels of theory, will depend on how wide of a context he wants to consider. If
he believes it necessary to use only
a limited area of experience (beyond that of “common sense”), he may use what
is generally called a “low-level theory.” Of course, this is not the only factor (limitations based on intelligence,
range of experience, etc. are important), but
this is
how solutions are normally arrived at. If he has deeper concerns, he may use a
higher-level of theory.
Since our class is concerned
primarily with understanding non-Western belief systems, this understanding is
further illustrated through readings from Nuer Religion, by E. E. Evans‑
9
Pritchard. In
one of the interesting references to his work, we discuss his premise that, “A
theistic religion need be neither monotheistic nor
polytheistic. It may be both. It is the question of the level...of thought, rather than
of exclusive types of thought” (1956:316). If you
understand monotheism to be exclusive in a system of belief, this may sound
like a contradiction in terms. However, in a number of belief
systems, like that of the Nuer, one “high” god and many spirits can
and do play complementary roles. The “level of thought” referred
to by Evans-Pritchard would equate in our model to a “monotheistic god” at the “high”
level, and a “polytheistic” realm of spirits at the “low level” of theory
(which compares to the middle range of Hiebert’s model which is
excluded by Westerners).
In traditional belief systems, spirits
provide explanations in broad but clearly defined contexts. This vital worldview belief relates
primarily to one’s immediate community and environment, and to daily concerns. A supreme being, however, while
related to a higher-level of theory,
tends to provide a means of explanation which relates to theories of the origin
of life, the reason for existence,
etc.
An interesting validation of the triangle-shaped
model is that more and more is explained by less and less as one moves up toward the
peak of the triangle. The “mystical” or “1st level of theory” explains a broad range of issues which confront
people on the daily sensory level.
The “2nd level of theory” provides an even broader
explanation as thought is generalized
into fewer abstract categories.
Another illustration of the use of “high/low
levels of theory” varying with context is when, in non-Western systems, a sickness does not respond to herbal
treatments of various kinds. The
practitioner may re-diagnose and try another alternative, but if there is still
no result, the conclusion is that
there is something else in this sickness. In other words, the context provided by common sense is too limited
and there is a shift to “mystical thinking” to resolve the problem. The Western analogy might be
the use of prescription medications to cure
an illness, but if this has no benefit, one may shift to prayer to resolve the
problem.
Later in the course, another topic, closely related to
the understanding that has just been developed, deals with the
comparison of “high” and “low religion,” to “Great” and “Little
Tradition.” I
generally use this model to explain the relationships:
10
FIG. 4
“High Religion” has been defined as “a
cognitive domain that is expressed in a highly institutionalized social organization”(
1. It deals primarily
with cosmic questions such as the origin and destiny of things, and the ultimate meaning of life.
2. It has written
texts that solidify or “fix” an authoritative body of beliefs. Since these texts are then unchangeable, as time passes,
commentaries must be written to make these meaningful and applicable to new times and changing cultures.
3. It is institutionalized.
This means a High Religion is characterized by specialization with different leadership and religious roles; it
has formally defined orthodox positions; it has central institutions such as temples or churches and schools for
training leaders, and it has bureaucratic
organizations of many forms.
4.
For the most part, “high
religion” provides for moral systems in which the gods (usually
male dominant) are good and in conflict with demons or “devils” who are evil.
By contrast, the category of “low religion” is a
cognitive domain in religion that is less institutionalized, and
centered more in immediate people-oriented needs and practices. Low
religion tends to have the following characteristics:
11
1.
It deals with the immediate problems of
everyday life... not ultimate matters. It is concerned with crisis,
disease, death, drought, etc.
2.
It is often “informally” organized. The
leaders may not even be specialists but only perform their religious services incidentally to their everyday work.
3.
It has no written texts. Beliefs are found
in myths, dramas and religious performances,
and since these are not “frozen” by being put into an unchanging form, they can change over time without the people being aware of it. So this
means they can be reinterpreted for
each new problem or occasion.
4.
A Low Religion is often amoral.
Emically, the spirits and beings in this world are understood
as being good or bad (more analogous to people). They help those who serve them
and harm those who forget them.
Somewhat
parallel to the model from R. Horton and B. Wilson is that of Robert Redfield.
Redfield was a professor at the
has taken a little different direction in relating
belief to social order, but it works itself out in
essentially the same way. If we focus with Redfield on the cognitive domain of High
Religion, especially in the context of
their social institutionalization, several levels of organization
can be discerned.
The Great Tradition refers
to great centers of organized religion, its central cathedrals, mosques, etc.,
training centers, formal bureaucracies, holy writings, and organizational
structures. It is here that we find the highest arenas for religious activity. Here
you have the clergy, the theologians, the leaders, and the scriptures of the
religion.
It is also important to know
that most of the books written about the world’s major religions deal
with the Great Tradition, giving extensive information about how it is rationalized,
delineating its systematic codes of beliefs, its commentaries, its
organization, etc. Most students who plan to work in second
cultures are directed to these kinds of sources.
They are, of course, important to give the ideal perspective of the
religion in question, but they give only a partial picture of the true nature
of actual belief.
The Little Tradition refers to the actual
expressions of the Great Tradition worked out in the daily lives of people at the “folk” level.
This is the religion of the average
12
believer...often
in the lives of people at the rural, peasant, or small village. A significant high level of the
population of most societies around the world function at
this level. Here there is a great deal of variance in the content, and even in
the structure of belief systems of the people, although the primary contrast is
between the Great and Little Tradition. Little Tradition
beliefs correlate poorly with what is defined as orthodox by the Great
Tradition. The Great Tradition tries
to control the lower levels, and determine for them what is orthodox
and correct, but in most areas of the non-Western world this is difficult. The
“fit” is better if channels of communication between the top
and the bottom are good, but this is most often not the case.
Redfield
also notes that ideas most often spread horizontally through the “folk” channels of communication across the Little
Tradition. They may also move up from the Little Tradition and find
acceptance in the Great Tradition, or they may be passed down from the Great to the Little Tradition.
Change is introduced in a number of ways, but it is usually very slow compared to changes in the
Western world. In addition, Redfield adds that the leaders at the level of Great Tradition may be unaware
of variations at Little Tradition level.
In teaching the course, I draw these and other concepts together, and consistently refer back
to The Excluded Middle in Western cognitive orientation. This middle
realm of belief, as we have seen, is important
and even vital to the belief systems of non-Western peoples, structurally and
practically. If we neglect it or disregard it, we will have potentially serious
problems in communicating the Gospel to people in other cultures. Having been
trained in Western education modes of thought, our automatic
reaction is to exclude this middle level and look for answers in other
categories of thought.
Historically,
and even today, when many cross-cultural workers enter a new society, many are not aware of the kinds of differences
that people make in categorizing religious beliefs. The workers focus on learning the language and proclaiming the
Gospel and generally assume God will
take care of the rest. An understanding of Religious Belief Systems is a tool in the process of contextualizing the Gospel in a new
society; but, it is a crucial tool. For example, there are some typical
mistakes to be seen as inferences are drawn from these various models:
1. Most Western-trained
missionaries and preachers, both having a Western cognitive
orientation centered in the “High Religion” realm, tend to concentrate
almost
13
entirely on issues of “High
Religion” which they believe are essential to bring about changes in the lives of people to whom they take
the Gospel. This is especially true if they have no training in cultural anthropology. Because "high
religion" is the central area of concern in their lives, and
because they are specialists in areas dealing with a high content of
theory, they unconsciously expect that to be the focal point to be dealt with
in the lives of others. Often, their communication is framed in “high
religion” sorts of concepts...primarily in
theological language, a major component of their own education. Even thought this may be “simplified” by those
working in a second culture, at least by their standards, it often seems
irrelevant to the daily lives and needs of average people.
2.
Western-oriented people often do not notice the Low Religion of a
particular area, or if they do, they
usually discount its importance. In the scientifically-trained worldview of the Western missionary, hearing that
sickness is caused by witchcraft or evil spirits seems like superstitious nonsense. He knows that germs cause
sickness. Even though he knows the
Bible teaches the interrelationship of the seen and unseen world of spirits, and though intellectually he believes
the Bible, he is conditioned in his automatic responses to such phenomena by his own scientific
training. He mentally takes the supernatural of low religion and puts it into the natural
category of his own Western belief system,
or excludes it entirely as “superstition.” Of course this is mainly due to the “excluded middle” in the Western worldview and
the failure to understand the importance of that entire area in the lives of non-Westerners which is so
important to them.
Obviously the form of
Christianity that comes to non-Western peoples often
fails to answer the questions raised in their daily lives. These questions were
answered by their old low
religion. For this reason, new converts usually turn back to the old religion to meet these needs. Or they may
take some religious symbol from the new religion, like the cross, and treat it like the old low religion symbols
are used (e.g., as magical).
There are some uncomplicated
solutions to these problems; however, they have profound
implications. It must be emphasized that:
1.
Cross-cultural workers must communicate in the forms of Christianity
that speak to these immediate, daily
real needs. It is usually much more appealing to people in “traditional” or “folk” cultures to relate the Gospel to points of need they
feel now, than to begin with the
cosmological issues of belief found in the “High Religion” domain of
14 Christianity. The
Bible does speak to those felt (and deeper true) needs, and those
biblical
solutions
must be studied, learned in an emic context, and applied in new
cultures.
2. Cross-cultural workers must
discover the cognitive domains of explanation in particular
societies and speak to all of those domains. If they only work with one area, viz.,
the “High Religion,” people will be only “partially converted.”
Many, if not most, “Christians” in the non-Western
world seem to be “High Religion” converts, but functioning
“folk religionists.” Lack of understanding on the part of Western cross-cultural
communicators of the intricacies of other belief systems may be at least
partially responsible for this very large and continuing
problem among people who claim to be Christians in every society
around the world.
What I have described thus far deals with issues that are
at the core of this course, but, as I noted at the beginning
of this paper, there is much more. The topics we cover in class, and through assigned readings
are:
(1)
History of the Anthropological Study of Religious
Belief Systems
(2)
Animism-The
Starting Point
(3)
The Flaw of the Excluded Middle
(4)
Western Beliefs vs. Traditional
(5)
Witchcraft, Sorcery and Magic
(6)
Religious/Magical Practitioners
(7)
Nuer Religion
(8)
Worldviews & Kinship-Based Cultures
(9)
Sins:
Cultural or Theological
(10) Symbolism and Ritual
(11) Power Concepts
(12) Spirit Possession
(13) Power Encounter
(14) Kalabari Worldview
(15) High and Low Religion
(16) Cognitive Categories
& Conversion
I also use several videos to
give a visual representation of what is primarily theoretical to these
students who have a limited cross-cultural experience.
The course papers done by
students in this class have given some surprising results of what
they have learned and applied in the course, many of them in ways that take the
application of this knowledge beyond what they are
expected to grasp in this undergraduate class.
As the course progresses, students are given more complex case studies and demonstrate
models of those belief systems, adding more content to the simple model with which
we begin. They are encouraged to think through the intricacies of the specific
system
15
they are studying and to diagram and analyze the belief
system as carefully as they can. Their final
course paper must include the model they construct and explain the rationale
for the way elements are placed in the
model.
My objective today is to give you a taste
of what we teach at
Listening to a former student of mine, now a
missionary, speaking of his work among the
Fulani in
I believe that as students of Religious Belief
Systems communicate the Gospel in other cultural frames of reference, they will be able to better understand
systems of thought and belief as they
work within the worldviews of the people they have gone to serve. I trust that you will consider the benefits of such training
for all students in ministry, especially those planning to be cross-cultural
workers.
**********
1Any
analysis should be done on the basis of and understanding of culture from the
inside. This insider’s viewpoint is known as an emic perspective.
The perspective of the informed outsider is called an etic perspective.
The terms were developed by linguist, Kenneth Pike, for
many years associated with the Wycliffe Bible Translators, and are now widely
used by anthropologists.
2General
systems theory grew out of the work of Austrian biologist Ludwig von
Bertalanffy (1901-1972). It encouraged anthropologists to
examine cultures as systems composed of both
human and non-human elements. It presumed that the normal state of a system was
equilibrium and described the various methods by which systems deviated from
states of balance and were returned to them. Roy A. Rappaport (1926-1997) was a
major proponent (cf. “Ritual Regulation of
Environmental Relations Among a New Guinea People”).
Other
16
background is
given in Understanding Folk Religion, P. Hiebert, R.D. Shaw, and T. Tienou,
chapter 2.
3Here I am referring to what is known as the
“psychological functionalism” of Bronislaw Malinowski. Briefly stated, the theory is that cultural institutions
function to meet the basic physical
and psychological needs of people in a society.
4A sub-category of cultural anthropology known as
“Cognitive Anthropology” has helped to
formulate some of my thinking, however a weakness of
this model, which I believe to be very
important, is its lack of emphasis on the use of historical contexts in this
type of analysis.
5The following statements are drawn largely from African
Traditional Thought and Western
Science, by Robin Horton, in Rationality,
Bryan R. Wilson, ed. and from Patterns of Thought in Africa and the West, by Robin Horton (See bibliography for full
references). 6From Kenneth Blackwell, speaking at the
17
BIBLIOGRAPHY BOOKS:
Awolawu, J. Omasade
1979 YORUBA BELIEFS
Longman
Group.
Bacon, Betty
1979 SPIRITISM IN
Bastide, Roger
1978 THE AFRICAN RELIGIONS OF
Sebba.
Berger, Peter, and Thomas Luckmann
1966 THE SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION OF REALITY: A TREATISE
IN THE
SOCIOLOGY
OF KNOWLEDGE. Garden City: Doubleday and
Company.
1983 THE BOOK OF CHINESES BELIEFS.
Bong Rin Ro
1985 CHRISTIAN
ALTERNATIVES TO ANCESTOR PRACTICES.
Burnett, David
1988 UNEARTHLY POWERS: A CHRISTIAN PERSPECTIVE ON PRIMAL
Davis, Wade
1985 THE SERPENT
Books.
Douglas, Mary
1973 RULES
1966 PURITY
OF POLLUTION
Paul.
Evans-Pritchard, E. E.
1956 NUER RELIGION.
1965 THEORIES OF
PRIMITIVE RELIGION.
Press.
1976 WITCHCRAFT, ORACLES
AZANDE.
Fortes, M. and G. Dieterien
1965 AFRICAN SYSTEMS
OF THOUGHT.
University Press.
Frazer, James G.
1922 THE GOLDEN BOUGH.
Geertz, Clifford
1967 ISLAM OBSERVED: Religious Developments in
1969 THE RELIGION OF JAVA.
van Gennep, Arnold
18
1977
THE RITES OF PASSAGE.
Harwood, Alan
1970 WITCHCRAFT, SORCERY,
AMONG THE SAFWA.
International African Institute.
Hicks, David
1976 TETUM GHOSTS
Publishing.
Hiebert,
Paul
1976 CULTURAL
ANTHROPOLOGY.
1985 ANTHROPOLOGICAL
INSIGHTS FOR MISSIONARIES.
1993 Evangelism, church and
kingdom. In THE GOOD
NEWS OF THE KINGDOM.
Charles Van Engen, Dean S. Gillaland, and Paul Pierson, eds.
1994 ANTHROPOLOGICAL REFLECTIONS
ON MISSIOLOGICAL ISSUES.
1999 THE MISSIOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF EPISTEMOLOGICAL SHIFTS.
Hiebert,
Paul, R. Daniel Shaw, and Tite Tienou
1999 UNDERSTANDING
FOLK RELIGION.
Books.
Horton, Robin
1993 PATTERNS OF THOUGHT IN AFRICAN
Essays on Magic, Religion, and Science.
1974 MAGIC, FAITH,
Kluckhohn,
1967 NAVAJO
WITCHCRAFT.
Kraft,
Charles H.
1981 CHRISTIANITY IN CULTURE: A STUDY IN DYNAMIC
BIBLICAL THEOLOGIZING IN CROSS-CULTURAL
PERSPECTIVE.
1989 CHRISTIANITY WITH POWER.
NY: Orbis Books.
2001 CULTURE,
COMMUNICATION
Kraft, Marguerite G.
1995 UNDERSTANDING
SPIRITUAL POWER.
Orbis Books. Levi-Strauss,
Claude
1963 TOTEMISM.
1979 MYTH
Loewen,
Jacob A.
2000 THE BIBLE IN CORSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE.
19
Luria, A.R.
1976 COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT: ITS CULTURAL
SOCIAL FOUNDATIONS.
Luzbetak, Louis
1963
THE CHURCH
Publications. Reprinted,
Malinowski, Bronislaw C.
1954 MAGIC, SCIENCE,
Doubleday.
[Original 1925]
Mayers, Marvin
1974 CHRISTIANITY CONFRONTS CULTURE.
1967 GODS
1967 MAGIC, WITCHCRAFT,
1967 MYTH
Musk, Bill A.
1984 POPULAR ISLAM: AN INVESTIGATION INTO
THE
PHENOMENOLOGY
1995 TOUCHING THE SOUL OF ISLAM: SHARING THE GOSPEL IN MUSLIM
CULTURES. Crowborough: MARC.
McGee, R. Jon, and Richard L. Warms
2000
ANTHROPOLOGICAL THEORY: AN INTRODUCTION
2000 EVANGELICAL DICTIONARY OF
WORLD MISSIONS.
Nida, Eugene
1960 MESSAGE
Parrinder, Geoffrey
1967
AFRICAN MYTHOLOGY.
1970 WITCHCRAFT: EUROPEAN
1991 AFRICAN DIVINATION SYSTEMS.
Phillips, Herbert P.
1966 THAI PEASANT PERSONALITY: THE PATTERNING OF INTER-PERSONAL
BEHAVIOR IN THE VILLAGE OF
BANG
CHAN. Lost Angeles:
Redfield, R.
1973
THE LITTLE COMMUNITY
Smalley, W.A.
20
1974
Spiro, Melford E.
1974 BURMESE SUPERNATURALISM.
Stephen, Michelle (Ed.)
1987 SORCERER
Steyne, Philip M.
1989 GODS OF POWER: A
STUDY OF THE BELIEFS
1967
1987 INTRODUCTION TO MISSIOLOGY.
Library.
Turner, Victor W.
1967 THE
1969 THE RITUAL PROCESS. Ithica:
1966 RELIGION: AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW.
Whiteman, Darrell
1980 MELANESIANS
HISTORICAL STUDY OF SOCIO-RELGIOUS CHANGE IN THE
SOUTHWEST PACIFIC.
Wilson, Bryan R. (Ed.)
1970 RATIONALITY.
JOURNAL ARTICLES:
Dye, T. Wayne
1976 Toward a Cultural Definition of Sin. Missiology
4:1. Hiebert, Paul G.
1982 The Flaw of the Excluded Middle. Missiology
10:35-47. Horton, Robin
1962
The Kalabari World View: An outline and interpretation.
Journal
of the International African Institute 32:3:197-219. 1968 African Traditional Thought and Western
Science I & II.
37:50-102.
Kraft, Charles H.
1963 Christian Conversion or
Cultural Conversion. Practical
Anthropology 10(4):179-187.
1973 Cultural Concomitants of Higi Conversion: Early
Period. Missiology 4:431-42.
Loewen,
Jacob A.
1960 Identification
and the Missionary Task. Practical
Anthropology: 7(1):1-15.
Maynard, Ashley E.
2003 Problems of
Translation. In ANTHROPOLOGY NEWS
Miller, Elmer S.
1973 The Christian
Missionary: Agent of Secularism. Missiology 1:99-108.